Research Article |
Corresponding author: Mark E. Hauber ( mhauber@gc.cuny.edu ) Academic editor: Ingolf Kühn
© 2024 Tomas Grim, Roi Dor, Mark E. Hauber.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Grim T, Dor R, Hauber ME (2024) Adaptive patterns of anti-predator escape behavior in a globally introduced bird species. NeoBiota 93: 143-156. https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.93.121380
|
Introduced species can represent quasi-experimental, anthropogenic case studies of both ecological and evolutionary principles. When these species are firmly established, competitive interactions between native and introduced species, including foraging, spacing, and breeding competition, may be among the ecological costs incurred from such species invasions. In turn, genetic and/or plasticity-driven changes in behavior and morphology could also take place in the invading species with increasing introduction lag (time since the onset of introduction). Critically, however, introduction lag is difficult to study in any single non-native population without long-term observations, and, instead, it requires geographically repeated measures of the focal response variables across invasive populations that were introduced at different times. Here we tested a priori predictors of predator-avoidance behaviors through the flight initiation distance (FID) assay of a widely distributed invasive bird species, the common myna Acridotheres tristis. The species was extensively and consistently sampled throughout most of its independently introduced ranges across all hemispheres. Critically, FID increased with greater introduction lag. We also detected additional functional patterns in that FID increased towards the rural range within a continuous metric of urban-rural gradient and also at shorter distances from the Equator. Any robust study of FID must also include proximate predictors as well and, accordingly, we found that FID increased with greater starting distance, with lower immediate human density, with flighted over walking escape responses, and at lower heights of a bird’s perch above ground but was unrelated to myna group size. Respectively, these factors are informative about the sensory cues triggering anti-predator behaviors in invasive mynas and imply an adaptive set of patterns of anti-predator responses in the introduced ranges of this species. Control measures of invasive common myna populations should take into account their extensive behavioral and cognitive flexibilities and adjust the planned management methods accordingly.
Anti-predator behavior, Indian mynahs, invasion latency, plasticity
Introduced species represent one of the major anthropogenically mediated global-change factors that threaten and reduce native biodiversity (
The common myna represents a successfully urbanized species in its native range (a Malaysia) and its flexible ecology allows it to successfully colonize even rural areas on isolated oceanic islands where it was released by humans (b Tahiti). Invasive animal species often share similar, especially urban, habitats (c New Zealand; in this case with introduced House Sparrows, Passer domesticus) and are often found in invasive vegetation (d Madagascar; in this case non-native pines Pinus sp.). Common mynas can reach very high local densities (e Madagascar; a small section of an 800-strong roosting flock), also due to their ability to use novel, anthropogenic food sources (f Israel). Photo credits: T. Grim.
Understanding how an invasive species adjusts to the novel environmental conditions, and especially whether invasion success is enhanced in a world with globally increasing urbanized habitats (e.g.,
Prior work on mynas has identified both morphological shifts along urbanization gradients within their introduced ranges (e.g.,
If mynas behave so as not to treat some evolutionarily novel heterospecifics as potential predators (e.g., because unexperienced mynas have not yet evolved to recognize them specifically, i.e. evolutionary lag), then we predict that, functionally, we can detect lower FIDs at sites with shorter introduction lags. Indeed, in a prior study, mynas at recently invaded areas showed less neophobia compared to both sites with longer invasion histories and within their native ranges (
Additionally, we tested several proximate factors, including starting distance, height of perch above ground, myna group size, immediate human density, and the escape modality (running vs. flying away) as sensory predictors of FID (sensu
Common mynas are native to South Asia, have been introduced to novel sites on five continents, including on several Pacific Island archipelagos, either as accidental releases of captive birds or deliberately imported agricultural control agents (
FID data on common mynas were collected consistently by a single observer (TG), thus avoiding any potential inter-observer bias. A total of n=451 FID data points were collected across much of the invasive ranges of the mynas (Fig.
Sampling locations (red crosses) for FID data outside the native (blue regions) and within the introduced (yellow regions; note that yellow color areas denoting Kuwait and Tahiti are too small and overlaid by red crosses marking sampling sites) ranges of common mynas. The distribution map was modified from
Most specific locations included in the present study were visited only once, thus preventing any potential pseudoreplication (repeated sampling of the same individuals). In a few cases the observer did not manage to sample the whole locality (park, botanic garden, etc.) during one visit or to reach reasonable sample size; in such cases another visit was done but a different part of the same locality was checked. We accounted for these patterns statistically (see below).
To assign a quantitative score of each locality’s urban-rural gradient at our study sites, we followed the urbanization assessment methodology of
Finally, to assign year of introduction to each locality, we sourced city or regional-level data from
We first generated two sets of standard least square mixed models, with restricted maximum likelihood (REML method), to assess first, 1A) the impact of all proximate predictors that we have collected upon the FID in mynas, using FID as the response metric and starting distance, height, and escape mode as predictors, with location identity (locality and place combined: column “loc_ID” in our data set available online at Figshare.com; see below) included as a random effect, for all data points collected across the study. Second, we analyzed a reduced sample-size model from the more recent years’ data points only to assess the impact of 1B) both all the previous and the additional proximate predictors upon FID in mynas, using FID as the response metric and starting distance, group (flock) size, height, immediate human density, and escape mode as predictors, with location identity (locality and place combined: column “loc_ID” in our data set which is available online at Figshare.com; see below) included as a random effect. Finally, we generated a model to assess the impact of 2) all functional predictors collected upon FID in mynas, using starting distance, altitude, absolute latitude (to account for the distance from the Equator for both northern and southern hemisphere locations), urbanization score and introduction lag as predictors, and country included as a random effect. In order to fully account for all measured parameters, only saturated (i.e., full) models incorporating all fixed effects were constructed for hypothesis testing and effect estimation. No model selection techniques were employed that could potentially exclude relevant explanatory variables. This was also an appropriate approach because none of the predictors within each model were strongly correlated with each other (all pairwise |r| < 0.2).
We included starting distance in both model types because it is the single best predictor of FID in prior studies (e.g.,
We did not log-transform the FID data in either model because doing so did not improve the W statistics of the Shapiro-Wilks test and so we also plotted the raw data in our figures. We used JMP 12.0 (SAS, Cary, NC, USA) for all statistical analyses and set alpha < 0.05. The data are made available through the following link: https://figshare.com/s/485def8f8e1b851e8539.
For the full data set (1A), we found that variation in FID was statistically significantly explained by all three predictors of our proximate factor model (R2 = 0.80): FID increased with greater starting distance, decreased at lower focal subject heights, and was greater when the myna escaped by flight (Table
Statistical outputs of the full proximate factor models’ impact on myna Flight Initiation Distance (FID).
Predictor | Estimate | Standard Error | F-statistics | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Starting Distance | 0.13 | 0.01 | F1,405 = 77.0 | P < 0.0001 |
Focal Subject Height | -0.38 | 0.12 | F1,407 = 9.84 | P = 0.0018 |
Escape Modality | -0.83 | 0.22 | F1,407 = 14.06 | P = 0.0002 |
For the more recent data set (1B), we found that variation in FID was again statistically significantly explained by most of the predictors of our proximate factor model (R2 = 0.80): FID increased with greater starting distance (Fig.
Relationships between Flight Initiation Distance (FID, in meters) and proximate predictors. These include A starting distance B immediate human density C height D escape mode, and E group size. Raw data are plotted in all figures, instead of the model predicted leverages. When fitted in a full mixed model, starting distance, urbanization, absolute latitude and intro- duction lag (the last three predictors shown in Fig.
Statistical outputs of the more recent years’ proximate factor models’ impact on myna Flight Initiation Distance (FID).
Predictor | Estimate | Standard Error | F-statistics | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Starting Distance | 0.13 | 0.02 | F1,355 = 65.02 | P < 0.0001 |
Immediate Human Density | -0.09 | 0.03 | F1,388 = 9.07 | P = 0.0028 |
Focal Subject Height | -0.40 | 0.13 | F1,354 = 9.82 | P = 0.0019 |
Escape Modality | -0.70 | 0.25 | F1,354 = 8.13 | P = 0.0046 |
Myna Group Size | -0.08 | 0.17 | F1,350 = 0.22 | P = 0.6380 |
We found that FID was significantly explained by most of the predictors of our functional factor model (R2 = 0.29): FID increased with introduction lag (Fig.
Relationships between Flight Initiation Distance (FID, in meters) and functional predictors. These include A urban-rural gradient B absolute latitude C altitude, and D introduction lag. Raw data are plotted in all figures, instead of the model predicted leverages. When fitted in a full mixed model, starting distance (shown in Fig.
Statistical outputs of the more recent years’ proximate factor models’ impact on myna Flight Initiation Distance (FID).
Predictor | Estimate | Standard Error | F-statistics | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Introduction Lag | 0.03 | 0.01 | F1,157 = 5.24 | P = 0.0234 |
Distance from Equator | -0.20 | 0.05 | F1,386 = 14.70 | P < 0.0001 |
Altitude | -0.002 | 0.001 | F1,183 = 2.45 | P = 0.1190 |
Urbanization | 0.92 | 0.16 | F1,433 = 32.62 | P < 0.0001 |
Starting Distance | 0.22 | 0.02 | F1,444 = 114.56 | P < 0.0001 |
With a multi-continent distribution of its native and introduced ranges, both long-term and short recency of repeated establishment, and high levels of aggression and competitive success over other cavity nesting birds, the common myna represents one of the most successful and globally impactful avian invaders (
Yet, an ecologically validated assessment of locally adaptive diversity in myna phenotypes has been still missing across the species’ diverse introduced ranges; we aimed to fill this knowledge gap using the flight initiation distance (FID) assay as a well-established anti-predatory ecological paradigm (see Introduction). We found that, at the functional level, introduction lag was moderately but positively associated with FID, implying more tolerance of human disturbance and perceived predation in the more recent invasions. However, introduction lag was moderated by both distance from the Equator, with positive impacts on FID only within the tropics and negative impacts on it in the temperate zones. In turn, FID increased with introduction lag in rural areas and decreased in urban areas. These interactions are consistent with a differential impact of local predation pressure as a function of local establishment latency in an invasive prey species (e.g.,
These functional patterns parallel previous findings, namely that at the forefront of invasions (compared to core invasion areas and native ranges), mynas show behavioral syndrome traits that include less neophobia and more exploration in their foraging behaviors (
Proximate cues for the common myna’s FID responses may represent sufficient sensory information that is available for this medium sized avian prey species to make decisions about escape responses from potential predators, represented by approaching humans in the FID assay. Our findings support this sensory hypothesis, in that several proximate factors directly predicted the FID in this data set: specifically, the further the observer started to directly approach the myna(s), the earlier the subject(s) responded to the threat (leading to a greater FID) (see also
In contrast, two of our proximate factors were related to FID in the opposite way from what we had predicted. With increasing immediate human density, FID was smaller, rather than greater (see
Similarly, we predicted that smaller FID would be associated with flying away escape responses (rather than running away responses), but we found the opposite to hold in our data set. This might be related to individual mynas’ behavioral syndrome: perhaps more easily flushed individuals are also more likely to escape on wing. We lack repeated observations of the same, marked individuals, thus we cannot assess other aspects of myna personalities, such as the repeatability of the escape style and FID consistency within the same individual. These topics deserve more research attention in the future. Finally, we detected no effect of myna group size on FID responses (similar to
Our findings demonstrate the flexibility of the common myna to adjust its behavioral responses to novel environmental conditions, including near-human habitats in an increasingly urbanized world. Smaller FID measured at sites of more recent invasions, of greater urbanization, more proximity to humans, may represent rapid habituation to people’s presence. This may also have implications for conservation control measures of the invasive populations. Specifically, myna management schemes and methods selection should take into account this behavioral flexibility, (e.g., the decrease in boldness with invasion time), and adjust the methods accordingly in less urbanized areas and in areas with longer invasions to achieve a more efficient outcome (see also González-Lagos et al. 2021).
Overall, our globally extensive and methodologically consistent study represents an opportunity to step beyond the multifold independent introduction history of a globally invasive species. Spatially isolated non-native populations allow independent tests of ecologically-relevant adaptive scenarios about behavioral tactics and strategies that would be often hard or impossible to test in native populations only (e.g.,
For funding, we are grateful to the USA-Israeli Binational Science Foundation (grant #2017258 to RD and MEH) and the Human Frontier Science Program (grants to TG and MEH). For calculating the urbanization score data, we thank Dr. Gabor Seress. For discussions and generating the map, we are grateful to Dr. Tali Magory Cohen. For editorial comments we are grateful to Dr. Ingolf Kuhn and referees.
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Data were collected only on public land and the methods of the study were approved by IACUC Protocol #17259 at the University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.
This work was supported by the USA-Israeli Binational Science Foundation (grant #2017258 to RD and MEH) and the Human Frontier Science Program (grants to TG and MEH).
TG: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Data collection; Writing – editing. RD: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Data curation and presentation; Writing – editing. MEH: Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Data curation, analyses and presentation; Writing – first and subsequent drafts.
Tomas Grim https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9517-9466
Roi Dor https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8743-9387
Mark E. Hauber https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2014-4928
Data are made available to the public through Figshare.com at the following link: https://figshare.com/s/485def8f8e1b851e8539.