Research Article |
Corresponding author: Szymon Sniegula ( szymon.sniegula@gmail.com ) Academic editor: Emili García-Berthou
© 2025 Szymon Sniegula, Dorota Konczarek, Maciej Bonk, Andrzej Antoł, Nermeen R. Amer, Robby Stoks.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Sniegula S, Konczarek D, Bonk M, Antoł A, Amer NR, Stoks R (2025) Non-consumptive effects of native, alien and invasive alien crayfish on damselfly egg life history and carry-over effects on larval physiology. NeoBiota 97: 215-235. https://doi.org/10.3897/neobiota.97.139760
|
Invasive alien (IA) predators pose significant threats to native ecosystems, often leading to profound impacts on prey species through both direct and non-consumptive effects (NCE). This study focused on the NCE of predator-induced stress from one native crayfish species, noble (Astacus astacus), compared to one alien danube crayfish (Pontastacus leptodactylus) and two IA crayfish species, signal (Pacifastacus leniusculus) and spinycheek crayfish (Faxonius limosus), on the native damselfly Ischnura elegans. We investigated the direct crayfish cue effect on egg traits as well as potential carry-over effects from the egg stage to the larval stage. We hypothesised that native crayfish cues would lead to more pronounced negative effects on prey traits compared to alien and IA crayfish, due to an evolutionary history of interaction and recognition of these threats. Unexpectedly, compared to native crayfish cues, alien and IA crayfish cues caused significantly higher egg mortality and prolonged developmental times, particularly cues from danube and signal crayfish, while cues from spinycheek crayfish had weaker, yet, still significant effects. Hatching synchrony was reduced and this to the same extent by the cues of all four crayfish species. Notably, cues from both alien and IA crayfish species caused significant carry-over effects, resulting in reduced larval survival, mass and fat content, which were more pronounced for danube and signal crayfish. Native crayfish cues did not induce carry-over effects, suggesting that I. elegans may have evolved a degree of resilience against this predator or that native crayfish produce chemical cues that do not cause a strong antipredator response. Our findings underscore the importance of considering immediate and carry over effects of crayfish on prey traits across multiple life stages, particularly in the context of biological invasions.
Carry-over effect, invasive alien species, life history, phenotypic plasticity, physiology, predator-prey interaction
In natural systems, ecological stressors mediated by human activity such as the introduction of invasive alien (IA) predators can drastically affect native prey populations, cascading to changes in the dynamics of native prey communities (
Predators can affect prey through direct consumptive effects or indirect non-consumptive effects (NCE). Predators can impose NCE by visual and/or chemical cues (kairomones), with the latter being unintentionally synthesised and released to the environment (
Kairomones are typically considered the primary drivers of NCE; however, predators can also host epibionts or promote microbial growth that may indirectly affect prey condition and survival, especially in aquatic systems (
While predator-induced changes in life history traits are well documented, effects of predators on physiological traits are less studied, yet also widespread (
NCEs might differentially affect prey traits during egg, larval and adult stages in prey with a complex life cycle (
In the studies on carry-over effects in general and especially for those related to predation risk, the egg stage has been understudied and typically only a limited number of egg traits have been considered. This may result in unmeasured or hidden carry-over effects of exposure to predation risk from the egg stage. The egg stage is a key window where the consequences of individual experiences can have lifelong effects on behaviour, physiology and fitness (
Here, we compare the NCEs imposed by native, alien and IA opportunistic omnivorous crayfish species (
Here, we focus on two fitness-related types of physiological traits, investment in immune function and energy storage, which have both been shown to be sensitive to predation risk in damselfly larvae (e.g.
Ischnura elegans is one of the most common native damselfly species in central Europe. It lives in a variety of freshwater habitats, including lentic and lotic waterbodies (
We studied two crayfish species that are native to Europe: the noble crayfish (Astacus astacus) and the danube crayfish (Pontastacus leptodactylus). The noble crayfish species is listed as vulnerable in Europe on the IUCN Red List (
Phylogenetic relationships amongst the studied crayfish species (modified from
The two studied IA crayfish species were the spinycheek crayfish (Faxonius limosus) and the signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus), which are both native to North America. The spinycheek species has been introduced to central Europe at the end of 19th century and is currently the most common crayfish in EU countries, including Poland and the study region specifically (
Phylogenetically, noble and danube crayfish are sister taxa and signal crayfish is from the same clade. In contrast, the spinycheek represents a different family and at the phylogenetic level is equally distanced from noble/danube and signal crayfish (
Copulating adult female Ischnura elegans were collected using a butterfly net on 15 June 2021 from two nearby ponds in the city of Krakow, Poland: Mydlniki ponds (50°05'09.6"N, 19°50'21.8"E) and Bonarka pond (50°01'25.4"N, 19°57'06.5"E). We selected these ponds because they supported numerous I. elegans populations. Additionally, the availability of historical and current crayfish distribution data allowed us to explore possible effects of pond-specific history of crayfish and population differences in damselfly responses to chemical cues. Mydlniki ponds are sourced by the Rudawa River that holds native noble crayfish (A. Klaczak 2023, pers. comm). To our knowledge, no crayfish have been recorded in Bonarka pond. However, in a nearby pond (approx. 350 m away), the danube crayfish was recorded until 2019 (M. Bonk 2019, unpublished). This absence of crayfish in Bonarka pond suggests that damselflies from this site may not have co-evolved with crayfish predators, potentially resulting in increased stress responses to both native, alien and IA species. The uncertainty regarding crayfish presence in Mydlniki ponds might also contribute to naïve responses. On the other hand, strong gene flow between I. elegans populations, as shown in recent studies (
Field-collected female damselflies were put in plastic jars with moisturised filter paper for egg laying and transported by car to the laboratory at the Institute of Nature Conservation PAS in Krakow. Jars with females were placed in a room with a temperature of 22 °C and natural day light. In total, 19 females from Mydlniki pond and 12 females from Bonarka pond laid large (> 100 eggs/clutch) egg clutches between 16 and 17 June 2021. These clutches were used in the experiment.
All crayfish species were collected in the field and transported by car to the laboratory several weeks prior the start of the experiment. Noble and danube crayfish were collected from a private pond near the town Miejska Górka (51°39'13.2"N, 16°58'52.3"E), spinycheek crayfish were collected from an excavation pond in Kryspinów (50°02'56.8"N, 19°47'28.7"E) and signal crayfish were collected from Hańcza Lake (54°15'31.9"N, 22°48'51.9"E). Noble, spinycheek and signal crayfish were collected and housed with permissions from, respectively, General Directorate of Environmental Protection in Warsaw (per. DZP-WG.6401.147.2021.TŁ), Regional Directorate of Environmental Protection in Krakow (per. OP.672.4.2021.GZ) and Regional Directorate of Environmental Protection in Białystok and Krakow (per. WPN.6205.21.2020.ML and OP-I.672.8.2020.MK1).
The densities of crayfish in aquaria were based on the basal metabolic rate equations obtained for crayfish (
At egg laying, every clutch (= family) was divided into five treatment groups, with 20 eggs per family per treatment. At hatching, these five egg-treatment groups were further split into two larval-treatment subgroups: a control group or a crayfish-exposure group. In the control subgroup, larvae were not exposed to CACC, allowing us to test for carry-over effects of predator exposure during the egg stage. Larvae in the CACC subgroup received the same crayfish treatment as in the egg stage. This resulted in nine treatment groups: control(egg) – control(larva), noble(egg) – control(larva), noble(egg) – noble(larva), danube(egg) – control(larva), danube(egg) – danube(larva), spinycheek(egg) – control(larva), spinycheek(egg) – spinycheek(larva), signal(egg) – control(larva) and signal(egg) – signal(larva) group (Fig.
A scheme of the experimental method, showing egg and larval crayfish treatments and the traits measured 44 days after hatching. Filled arrows indicate carry-over non-consumptive effect (NCE), empty arrows indicate continues exposition to NCE. Abbreviations for the crayfish treatment groups are indicated to the right of the larval treatment groups: CC – control(egg)-control(larva), NC – noble(egg)-control(larva), NN – noble(egg)-noble(larva), DC – danube(egg)-control(larva), DD – danube(egg)-danube(larva), SC – spinycheek(egg)-control(larva), SS – spinycheek(egg)-spinycheek(larva), SiC – signal(egg)-control(larva) and SiSi – signal(egg)-signal(larva) group.
The number of larvae hatched per cup ranged from 2 to 34. The larvae were fed ad libitum daily with laboratory-cultured Artemia nauplii. When the earliest hatched larvae in each cup reached the age of 44 days, all larvae from the same cup were group-weighed and frozen in the same Eppendorf tube at -80 °C for physiological analyses. We chose this larval age for two reasons: it represented approximately 50% of the larval development time until emergence and each group had reached the minimal wet mass threshold for the analysis of physiological traits.
The proportion of eggs that survived per cup was calculated as the number of eggs per cup that hatched. The unhatched eggs were considered as dead. We noted the egg development time from egg laying to hatching. Every cup was checked for new hatchlings every morning and afternoon, with half a day used as the measurement unit. Based on the egg development times in a given cup, we estimated hatching synchrony per cup as the coefficient of variation (CV); the smaller the CV, the higher the hatching synchrony. This trait is relevant to measure because it can represent one of the preys’ tactics for escaping predation pressure, for example, predator satiation effect (
We assessed physiological traits from the body supernatants of preserved larvae. To prepare the body supernatant, the larvae were homogenised in PBS buffer (Phosphate-Buffered Saline, final mass × 15 µl PBS) and subsequently centrifuged.
As a measure of investment in immune function, we quantified the activity of phenoloxidase (PO). This enzyme plays a key role in the defence of insects against bacterial, fungal and viral agents (
We determined the fat content of damselfly larvae using a modified protocol based on Marsh and Weinstein (
All the tests were performed using R version 4.3.2. Following packages were used: the lme4 package for general linear mixed models (
The number of individuals considered in the analyses ranged from 86 to 339 (egg treatments) and from 5 to 10 (cumulative egg and larval treatments) per treatment combination. Suppl. material
The analyses showed significant main effects of CACC on egg and larval life history and physiological traits, as well as interacting effect of CACCs and pond on egg life history in I. elegans.
Family (random effect) explained 21.5% of the variance in egg development time and 17.7% in larval mass. Variance explained by family was lower for other traits and models for hatching synchrony and larval fat content indicated negligible family-level variance (model singularity). The variance explained by family was generally higher after accounting for fixed effects, such as CACC treatment and pond, which indicated the role of experimental treatments in shaping observed variability. A table summarising the variance explained by family is available in Suppl. material
In general, CACC had a negative effect on egg survival (Fig.
Effects of crayfish-associated chemical cues (CACCs), pond and their interactions on egg survival, development time in days and hatching synchrony measured as coefficient of variation (CV) in I. elegans. Significant p-values are in bold.
Predictor | df | Chisq | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Egg survival | |||
CACCs | 4 | 78.8 | < 0.001 |
Pond | 1 | 0.2 | 0.680 |
Egg development time | |||
CACCs | 4 | 1206.8 | < 0.001 |
Pond | 1 | 0.9 | 0.327 |
CACCs × pond | 4 | 10.8 | 0.028 |
Hatching synchrony, CV | |||
CACCs | 4 | 33.6 | < 0.001 |
Pond | 1 | 0.1 | 0.814 |
Effects of crayfish cues from native and invasive alien (IA) crayfish species on the egg survival rate (A), development time (B) and hatching synchrony (C) in I. elegans. Shown are means with 95% CI. Different letters indicate means that are significantly different, based on Tukey pairwise tests.
Overall, eggs took longer to develop under the CACC treatment. This result was especially pronounced under the signal CACC (+10 days), which caused the longest egg development time, followed by the danube (+7 days), spinycheek (+4 days) and noble (+2 days) CACC. These results were supported by Tukey’s HSD pairwise comparisons (Fig.
Hatching was about two times more synchronised under the control treatment than in the presence of CACC (Fig.
In general, exposure to CACCs decreased larval survival when quantified when the first larva in a cup reached an age of 44 days (Fig.
Effects of crayfish-associated chemical cues and pond on larval survival and mean values of mass, fat content and phenoloxidase activity (PO) per cup. Analyses on larval mass, fat content and PO were corrected by number of larvae per cup. Significant p-values are in bold.
Predictor | df | Chisq | p-value |
---|---|---|---|
Survival | |||
CACCs | 8 | 56.7 | < 0.001 |
Pond | 1 | 0.2 | 0.675 |
Mass | |||
CACCs | 8 | 67.6 | < 0.001 |
Pond | 1 | 0.1 | 0.732 |
No. of larvae per cup | 1 | 175.6 | < 0.001 |
Fat content | |||
CACCs | 8 | 22.3 | 0.004 |
Pond | 1 | 1.1 | 0.299 |
No. of larvae per cup | 1 | 2.7 | 0.102 |
Phenoloxidase activity | |||
CACCs | 8 | 10.9 | 0.211 |
Pond | 1 | 0.3 | 0.592 |
No. of larvae per cup | 1 | 31.4 | < 0.001 |
Effects of crayfish-associated chemical cues (CACCs) from native and invasive alien (IA) crayfish on the larval survival rate (A), mass (B), fat content (C) and phenoloxidase activity (D) in I. elegans. Note that in treatment combinations where the second letter is “C” (hence NC, DC, SC and SiC), the larvae were only exposed to the CACCs in the egg stage, but not in the larval stage, hence, when different from the control CC treatment would indicate a carry-over effect from egg exposure. Shown are means with 95% CI. Different letters indicate means that are significantly different, based on Tukey pairwise tests. Abbreviations for CACCs along the x-axis are as in Fig.
CACC decreased larval mass (Fig.
CACC negatively affected the total fat content (Fig.
CACC did not affect phenoloxidase activity (PO) (Fig.
We examined the non-consumptive effects (NCEs) of native, alien and invasive alien (IA) crayfish species on the egg and larval traits of the damselfly I. elegans, with special attention for potential carry-over effects from the egg to the larval stage. Our results do not support the first (prey naivety) hypothesis stating that alien and IA crayfish species exert weaker effects on damselfly egg survival and development time than native crayfish species. Instead, the strongest effects were observed in response to the IA signal crayfish-associated chemical cues (CACCs), which has not yet invaded the damselfly sampling ponds, supporting one of our alternative hypotheses. Eggs exposed to these cues exhibited the longest development time, lowest survival and disrupted hatching synchrony. While we did detect negative carry-over effects on larval survival and mass in response to egg exposure to CACCs, this was only the case for cues from one alien crayfish (danube) and one IA crayfish (signal), supporting the alternative hypothesis (i.e. the opposite of the prey naivety hypothesis). Interestingly, only egg exposure to IA signal CACCs induced a negative carry-over effect on larval fat content. These are important results for invasion biology as they suggest that crayfish-induced NCE cannot only persist across life stages, but are also not restricted to native predators. Finally, there was limited support for the last alternative hypothesis, as the observed responses did not strictly follow phylogenetic relationships. In other words, closely-related crayfish species did not consistently elicit similar prey responses across all traits assessed.
Our findings underscore the importance of studying egg-stage predator-prey interactions in species with complex life cycles, as exposure during the egg stage can significantly influence fitness-related traits. CACC from the IA signal crayfish reduced by half egg survival and extended the egg development time by 10 days, indicating that the mere presence of IA predator-associated chemical cues can induce strong stress responses in damselfly eggs. Such responses are consistent with other studies demonstrating that exposure to predator cues during the early life stages can trigger significant physiological changes that decrease egg survival (
Interestingly, CACC from the alien danube crayfish, which has been present in the region for over a century (
In contrast, native noble crayfish and locally invading IA spinycheek crayfish had no effect on egg survival. This suggests that the eggs from the studied damselfly populations may have evolved some resistance to the NCEs of these crayfish or that these species produce weaker CACCs that do not cause strong antipredator egg responses (
The eggs of I. elegans prolonged development times under exposure to CACCs and this across all treatment groups, yet, with significant differences between native, alien and IA crayfish. Eggs exposed to IA signal CACCs showed the longest delay, whereas native noble CACCs caused the shortest delay, but still significant. This variation suggests that damselfly eggs exhibit flexible plasticity in response to predation risk and that the imposed risk is the highest under IA crayfish (
The observed disruption in hatching synchrony under CACC exposure, with similar strength across all treatment groups, indicates that predator-associated cues may also affect egg cohort timing. Reduced hatching synchrony can have ecological implications, as it may reduce the effectiveness of antipredator strategies like predator satiation (
Our study showed significant carry-over effects from the egg stage to the larval stage when I. elegans eggs were exposed to CACCs from native, alien and IA crayfish species. Larvae that were only exposed to CACCs during the egg stage showed reduced survival, lower body mass and reduced fat content compared to control groups, indicating that predator-induced stress effects can persist across life stages. Notably, the strength of these carry-over effects varied amongst the three crayfish types, with the most pronounced negative effects observed for the alien danube and IA signal CACCs. This pattern aligns with previous research suggesting that alien and IA predators may elicit stronger stress responses due to the absence of evolutionary exposure of prey to these predators (
Our study also indicated that larvae exposed to CACCs during both the egg and larval stages exhibited greater reductions in mass and fat content than those only exposed in the egg stage. This cumulative effect of exposure to predator-associated chemical cues indicates that the stress induced during the egg stage was not softened after hatching and that continuous exposure further intensifies the negative effects. For instance, larvae that experienced noble, danube and signal CACCs during both stages showed significantly lower mass across all treatment groups. The negative effect of predator stress on prey mass or size was earlier shown in other damselflies species and semi-aquatic insects such as mayflies (
The significant reduction in larval fat content in response to CACCs as observed in our study provides further evidence that predator-induced stress can disrupt energy allocation across life stages. In semi-aquatic invertebrates, fat reserves are critical for sustaining growth and immune function during the larval stage (
Finally, we found no significant effect of predator-associated cues on phenoloxidase activity, our measure of investment in immune function. This happened probably because there was apparently no effect of continuous exposure and so no immediate effect, which may explain also the absence of any delayed effects. This absence of an effect on immune parameters may reflect the complex and variable nature of carry-over effects, where some traits, such as energy storage and total body mass, are more susceptible to early-life stressors than others.
Our study underscores the impacts of predator exposure in species with complex life cycles, where early-stage stressors can persist and negatively affect later stages. These findings are particularly relevant for predicting how non-native species, such as the IA crayfish, can alter prey populations through non-consumptive effects that accumulate over time. Given that alien and IA predators imposed stronger selective pressures on naïve prey, as found in terms of a higher effect on egg and larval survival, egg development time and larval mass and fat content, it is crucial to incorporate these carry-over and cumulative stress effects into ecological models to better predict population responses and ecosystem dynamics under biological invasions.
We would like to thank P. Ławniczak for providing access to the pond with crayfish and for his assistance in catching them.
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
No ethical statement was reported.
SS, AA, NRA acknowledge support from the National Science Centre, Poland (Grant number: 2019/33/B/NZ8/00521) and SS acknowledges support from the Institute of Nature Conservation Polish Academy of Sciences. RS acknowledges support from FWO Flanders.
Conceptualization: SS. Data curation: AA, SS. Formal analysis: SS. Funding acquisition: SS. Investigation: SS, AA, NRA, DK. Methodology: SS, RS, AA. Project administration: SS. Resources: SS. Supervision: SS. Visualization: NRA (Figure
Szymon Sniegula https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1459-3751
Maciej Bonk https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4093-2542
Andrzej Antoł https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9730-7417
Nermeen R. Amer https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7371-0028
Robby Stoks https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4130-0459
All of the data that support the findings of this study are available in the main text or Supplementary Information.
Supplementary data
Data type: docx
Explanation note: An additional figure showing the effects of crayfish cues and the pond of origin on egg development time in I. elegans, as well as tables showing the variance explained by family (random effect) for different response traits, number of individuals per treatment group and collection pond, and multiple comparisons of proportions using Tukey’s HSD contrasts.
Raw data used for the statistical analysis
Data type: xlsx